Roger, 

 

I was taught in graduate school that the ability to make and hear phomenes
of all language is inborn, but as you learn a language, you lose the ones
you don't need, and after about age six, it's hard (or impossible?) to get
them back.  I also heard that there was some language school in Boston that
goes at that problem head on and gets back those phonemes for you.   But
this is the sort of stuff you probably know more about than I do so I won't
say more. 

 

I am trying to sing German and Italian right now and both require a trilled
"r".  I simply cannot do it.  The closest I can manage is singing the German
r as a "d", which is close enough for gummint work, but very unsatisfying.
I also can't blow a proper raspberry.  Never have been.  

 

If you run into somebody in Santa Fe that recovers lost phonemes for old
people, let me know. 

 

Nick 

 

From: friam-boun...@redfish.com [mailto:friam-boun...@redfish.com] On Behalf
Of Roger Critchlow
Sent: Monday, February 13, 2012 12:04 PM
To: The Friday Morning Applied Complexity Coffee Group
Subject: Re: [FRIAM] FW: Re: Friam Digest, Vol 104, Issue 9

 

I was torturing myself yesterday listening to Spanish instructional material
in my car.  The tapes spent a lot of time presenting minimal contrasts
between vowels in different contexts, between consonants, or between
alternative stresses.  I can hear some of the contrasts quite clearly, I can
hear others if I listen carefully, but there are some where my auditory
system just goes "what?".  I'm hoping that reviewing the tapes with the text
in front of me will help, or that I was just getting tired.  I suspect that
someone with a differently accented american English upbringing might have
trouble with a different subset of the contrasts.

 

I think there's a parallel between all of this and
http://web.mit.edu/newsoffice/2012/error-correcting-codes-0210.html.  The
decoding of linguistic communication is an omnivorous error correcting
algorithm which operates in situations with unpredictable noise.  It will
use anything associated with the communication act to aid a decoding, but it
usually gets by with the simplest and most straightforward coding.  And it
decodes all the alternate code words, as it were, in parallel.  

 

So we're happy to decode scrambled words, as long as the word length and
letter distributions are correct and the scrambled words make a sensible
utterance.  Why not?  We're perceiving word lengths and letter distributions
all the time when we read.

 

Similarly, but harder to demonstrate in a typed email, my calligraphy
teacher once showed us that a line of text can be read from only the tops of
the letters.  The bottoms aren't nearly as informative.

 

-- rec --

 

On Mon, Feb 13, 2012 at 10:41 AM, Eric Smith <desm...@santafe.edu> wrote:

Eric, hi,

 

There is a (to me) fun similarity between this sequence you have
illustrated, and some pathologies and treatments in rapid auditory
processing, in which the workers I know are April Benasich and Paula Tallal
(this, from a few years ago).  

 

The stopped phonemes, particularly the voiced, non-plosives (in English, b,
d, g) differ by transients that are on the order of <<100ms in duration.  A
subset of children are born with deficits that render them unable to resolve
transients this short in the primary auditory cortex (I don't know or
remember what the right reference is to the brain region or layer).  As a
result, they can't hear difference between /bag/ and /gag/, and they grow up
with severe dyslexia through having never had needed queues to the structure
of the language.  

 

The human vocal tract can't alter these stops, because they are mechanically
determined, so there is nothing the caregiver can do.  What the remedy is,
is electronically altered speech, which can slow the transient down while
retaining the structure of the formant timeseries that distinguishes the
segments, so that it spans more than 100ms, while leaving the rest of the
word length and overall speech prosody the same.  The child is played a few
hours of this altered speech per day, through the early formative few years
(I forget, perhaps pre-9-month to 3 years).   The child develops near-normal
language and reading skills, and the altered-speech therapy can be phased
out.  After that, the child functions essentially normally.  In sentence
context, he believes he is hearing the distinction between /bad/ and /dad/,
and processes sentences normally, though in fact this rapid-auditory deficit
is congenital and never develops normal function.  However, there is so much
redundancy built into the combination of phonology, lexicon, morphology, and
syntax, that it can serve many functions over the course of lifetime
language use, from child learning, to error correction in normal speech, as
in noisy environments, or correcting for speaker accent variants.  Our
colleague Morten Christiansen even has evidence that phonotactic queues
provide significant evidence to distinguish word categories such as noun and
verb in English, which child language-learners are using, along with
morphological queues.  

 

Somehow, all this works, because there is a complicated mix of top-down
conditioning of fine-scale perceptual processing, updated by bottom-up
updating from the actual rapid inputs.  

 

I have assumed that things like this are also going on for written language,
and I have also assumed (though in this case without defense from
neuroscience), that this is an expression of a general feature of the
multi-level character of neural processing, which can also be seen in the
function of systems such as reflex arcs, which, e.g., require different
responses for reaching to touch, versus to grasp, even though they use the
same afferent and efferent systems, and are conditioned by a reflex arc
rather than going all the way back up to tactile/motor cortex interactions.

 

I don't know whether this addresses the kind of explanations you think would
be relevant or appropriate to phenomena such as this.  

 

All best,

 

Eric

 

 

 

On Feb 13, 2012, at 7:28 AM, ERIC P. CHARLES wrote:

 

Benny,

The first quote is the only one I think is from me. To clarify:

When you say that the phenomenon is "reading gibberish", then it seems like
it might be a skill. However, if you phrase it as a failure to distinguish
gibberish from properly written words, or as mistaking gibberish for
properly written words, then it seems less impressive. Let's set up a
hypothetical longitudinal study... 

At age two you show your child two words "amazing" and "azanmig" and
verbally ask them which one is the word "amazing". The child guesses at a
50/50 rate, and from that we conclude that the child in unskilled. Later, at
age 4, the same test is given, and carefully sounding out the words, the
child identifies the correct word every time. This, we conclude,
demonstrates growing reading skills. Next, lets say, thirty years later, we
send the child an email, in which, in the middle of a sentence, there is a
claim about the azanmig powers of the human mind. Our (adult) child reads
the sentence without even noticing that there is a mistake. Why would the
failure to perform at the level of a 4-year-old be suddenly taken as a sign
of advanced sophistication? 

Surely there is some way to argue that the final incident indicates an
advanced skill of some sort, but our initial bias should be to assume that
failures to discriminate indicates a lack of skill. 

Is that more clear?

Eric 



On Sun, Feb 12, 2012 06:15 PM, Benny Lichtner <benjamin_licht...@brown.edu>
wrote:

Hi, all. Just want to respond to some of these points as I am very
interested in meaning making.

 

It [the ability to read "gibberish"] does not demonstrate mysterious skill,
it demonstrates a (perhaps mysterious) lack of skill. The real mystery, if
there is one, is why a person so well trained in reading would be fooled by
such a simple manipulation. 

 

I do not see where the "lack of skill" is. Could you elaborate? What skill
is it that we lack because of our ability to read "gibberish"? Is it the
skill of identifying what is "gibberish" and what is not? The problem with
"gibberish" is that you always have to put scare-quotes around it.

 

I have to ask why even an intelligent person can make some kind of sense out
of ["]Gibberish.["] Something about our brains seems to Beg for Sense when
there is none. 

 

I suspect that were our brains not to "beg for sense," we would never be
able to read any non-"gibberish." There are no hard rules for generating
meaning, just some customs we use, and are used to, and break much of the
time.

 

We prefer magical explanations because they do not require any effort. 

 

Is it that we "prefer" magical explanations, or just that they are the first
to come to mind, and are perfectly satisfactory explanations at that, so why
look for others? If the audience were simultaneously presented with a
magical and a non-magical explanation, I am not sure which would be
preferred. It might have nothing to do with magicality. Maybe more to do
with creativity, or realism, depending on who you are.

 

All Physicists are apparently insane because they intentionally look for
difficult explanations. 

 

The explanations that physicists look for are difficult by coincidence, I
think. They look for explanations that fit their assumptions about knowledge
and causality, and those explanations happen to be difficult, both to
invent/discover and to understand.

 

Apparently Cryptic equals Important for most people.

 

I wonder if this has something to do with the reward that comes from
understanding something challenging. Cryptic (wo)man-made things may also
seem important because it is mysterious why a (wo)man would spend time
creating something cryptic.

 

--Benny Lichtner

Eric Charles

Professional Student and
Assistant Professor of Psychology
Penn State University
Altoona, PA 16601



============================================================
FRIAM Applied Complexity Group listserv
Meets Fridays 9a-11:30 at cafe at St. John's College
lectures, archives, unsubscribe, maps at http://www.friam.org

 


============================================================
FRIAM Applied Complexity Group listserv
Meets Fridays 9a-11:30 at cafe at St. John's College
lectures, archives, unsubscribe, maps at http://www.friam.org

 

============================================================
FRIAM Applied Complexity Group listserv
Meets Fridays 9a-11:30 at cafe at St. John's College
lectures, archives, unsubscribe, maps at http://www.friam.org

Reply via email to