Hi Liviu,

I can't help but weigh in on this one.  I think it really depends on who the 
audience is, how much they are motivated, and the medium through which you are 
delivering the message.

> My personal experience from the bench suggests something similar.
> Whenever I spend too much time trying to figure out what was written
> (say, on the blackboard) I have much less time to focus on what was
> meant.

This is my experience as well.

Making a lecture or presentation difficult to understand is a terrible, 
terrible idea.  Verbal communication is already inefficient (people speak much 
slower than they read) and our "working memory" for speech is smaller than it 
is for written text.  Combine it with the fact that people can only focus on 
one thing at a time and you have disaster.  If you become distracted reading 
text and miss what the speaker is saying, , you might miss something important. 
 If that something important is part of the logical chain of the argument (say 
you missed an important step in the derivation of a formula), it can greatly 
change how you understand everything that comes afterward.  These limitations 
are mitigated by the fact that presentations are interactive, which means that 
a good presenter can repeat and gauge the understanding of the audience in real 
time.

With written text, there is the advantage that you can deliver much more 
information and provide it at much higher resolution than you ever could in a 
presentation.  If there is a point of confusion, the reader simply returns to 
re-read earlier passages.

But density can also have its problems.  If the words on the page appear too 
dense, people will refrain from reading them unless there is some motivation to 
do otherwise.  It's important to note that it doesn't actually matter what the 
words say, if the line length is too long, or the word forms appear convoluted, 
or the layout is poor, it all has the same effect.  This paper does a pretty 
good job of discussing the ways that different layout features influence letter 
detection and readability.

Denis G. Pelli et al., “Feature detection and letter identification,” Vision 
Research 46, no. 28 (December 2006): 4646-4674.  

if you can't access the message of the article, it doesn't really matter what 
it says.

I agree with one point in the economist article, it is good to make people 
think deeply about things.  Challenging the mind and forcing it to process 
information will result in long-term retention.  All of the study's observation 
can probably be explained by one thing: the slightly more difficult text caused 
people to slow down as they read, taking more time to digest  But isn't that 
the point of  exercises, engaging stories and other rhetorical devices?

But the tolerances for "making the text harder to read" are probably so narrow, 
that the technique may as well be worthless.  At what point does effective 
become convoluted?  In all of his books, Tufte makes the point that "dense" 
does not mean "well designed."

I think it better to achieve the same result through other means.  Striking 
examples, well drawn illustrations, anecdotes, intelligent graphs can all 
accomplish exactly the same thing while keeping the text clean and readable.  
The goal should be to focus attention, not create additional work.

Just my two cents, though.

Cheers,

Rob

Reply via email to